Review: Reading Visual Narratives

No Comments

AUTHOR: Clare Painter
AUTHOR: James R. Martin
AUTHOR: Len Unsworth
TITLE: Reading Visual Narratives
SUBTITLE: Image Analysis of Children’s Picture Books
SERIES TITLE: Functional Linguistics
PUBLISHER: Equinox Publishing Ltd
YEAR: 2013

REVIEWER: Daniel Lees Fryer, University of Gothenburg

SUMMARY

Children’s picture books may be designed to delight and entertain young
readers (and adults), but they are also a key site for apprenticeship into and
sensitization to literacy, literature, and social values. In ‘Reading Visual
Narratives: Image Analysis of Children’s Picture Books,’ Claire Painter, J. R.
Martin, and Len Unsworth explore, through the lens of social semiotics, how
such books construct meanings and help scaffold learning through combinations
of visual and verbal resources.

‘Reading Visual Narratives’ is divided into five main chapters, and includes a
Preface, an acknowledgments section, a reference list, a picture-books
bibliography, and an index. Chapter 1, “Reading the Visual in Children’s
Picture Books,” starts by discussing the pedagogic and social significance of
these books. Painter et al. argue that developing analytic tools for
understanding the visual and verbal meaning-making resources in children’s
picture books will provide valuable insights for “literacy educators and
children’s literature specialists” (p. 2).

The book takes a social-semiotic and multimodal discourse analysis (MDA)
approach based on systemic-functional theory, with the dual aim of: 1)
understanding how individual picture books make meaning; and 2) extending
current social-semiotic accounts of the visual mode, both intra- and
intersemiotically. The authors note, in this introductory chapter, how their
work draws upon and extends that of Kress and van Leeuwen (1996, 2006),
particularly in terms of visual narrative and visual resources for negotiating
emotional engagement with the reader.

Additionally, Chapter 1 includes a review of previous work on children’s
picture books, from a variety of theoretical and educationally oriented
perspectives, including Nodelman’s (1988) ‘Words about Pictures.’ The chapter
also includes a short introduction to systemic-functional theory. This
introduction is based on Halliday (1978), Halliday and Matthiessen (2004),
Martin and Rose (2007), and Kress and van Leeuwen (2006), among others. Within
this theoretical framework, Painter et al. highlight ideational,
interpersonal, and textual meanings, the systemic-functional notion of system,
and the visualization of system networks.

The final section of Chapter 1 describes the material for the study, a corpus
of 73 “critically well-regarded” (p. 11) children’s picture books. The books
have varying proportions of words (verbiage) to images, and are aimed at a
wide range of age groups and reading abilities. All of the books are
English-language editions, and they span a publication period from 1902 (the
1902 edition of Beatrix Potter’s ‘The Tale of Peter Rabbit’) to 2008 (Anthony
Browne’s ‘Little Beauty’). Examples from many of these picture books are
reproduced throughout the book.

Chapter 2, “Enacting Social Relations,” examines the visual encoding of
interpersonal meanings in children’s picture books. The chapter begins by
briefly presenting Kress and van Leeuwen’s (2006) proposed systems for
construing relations between readers/viewers and represented participants or
other visual elements, i.e., the systems of SOCIAL DISTANCE, INVOLVEMENT,
POWER, CONTACT, and MODALITY (Systems are represented by small caps in Painter
et al., standard notation in many systemic-functional works; block caps are
used for the purposes of this review. Note also that square brackets are used
here and in Painter et al. to indicate options or features within a system.).
The authors then propose a number of complementary or alternative systems to
account for the interpersonal visual meanings relevant to children’s picture
books. These include FOCALIZATION, AFFECT, PATHOS, AMBIENCE, and GRADUATION.

FOCALIZATION considers point of view, and whether viewers are positioned as
having [contact] with represented participants (usually through the outward
gaze of a character in a book) or whether viewers take on an observer role
([observe], minus the gaze) (cf. Kress and van Leeuwen’s (2006) ‘demand’ and
‘offer’). Further simultaneous options concern how the reader’s point of view
is managed as either [mediated], i.e., as seen vicariously through the eyes of
a character, or as [unmediated], i.e., without being positioned as a
character. More delicate options are available that include variations in the
types of gaze ([direct] or [invited]) and mediation ([inscribed] or
[inferred]). The system of AFFECT, i.e., the depiction of characters’
feelings, is based on three basic styles of character depiction: minimalist,
generic, and naturalistic. These drawing styles engender varying levels of
reader engagement (or PATHOS), from [appreciative] (minimalist) and [empathic]
(generic) to [personalizing] (naturalistic), allowing different feelings or
affect to be read through a combination of facial expressions and bodily
postures. AMBIENCE accounts for the emotional mood created by the use of
color. Of particular note here are the simultaneous subsystems of VIBRANCY,
WARMTH, and FAMILIARITY, which provide various options for differing levels of
saturation, the graded use of [warm] and [cool] colors, and the degree to
which color differentiation encodes what is naturalistic or somehow [removed]
from reality. For GRADUATION, i.e., the upscaling or downscaling of evaluative
meanings (p. 44), the authors emphasize the need for further investigation,
but tentatively discuss how [quantification] can be scaled [up] or [down]
according to [number], [mass/amount], and/or [extent].

The chapter concludes with a visual interpersonal analysis of Raymond Briggs’s
(1994) ‘The Tin-Pot Foreign General and the Old Iron Woman.’ The authors
provide a descriptive analysis (pp. 46-49) and a table that summarizes,
page-by-page and system-by-system, the various interpersonal instantiations
and their realizations (pp. 50-52).

Chapter 3, “Construing Representations,” explores visual ideational meanings
in children’s picture books. Based on Kress and van Leeuwen’s (2006)
categories of represented participants, processes, and circumstances, Painter
et al. investigate visual ideational meanings through the proposed systems of
CHARACTER MANIFESTATION and APPEARANCE, CHARACTER RELATIONS, and INTER-EVENT
and INTER-CIRCUMSTANCE relations, in order to account for narrative sequences
in children’s picture books. CHARACTER MANIFESTATION and APPEARANCE deal with
the ways in which characters (or participants) are introduced into and tracked
across visual narratives, and the ways in which variations in character
attributes, such as [descriptive detail], [symbolic attributes], and clothing
and accessories ([garment/accessory]), are accounted for. The system of
CHARACTER RELATIONS deals with how images invite various comparisons of
characters and their attributes, for example, through the juxtaposition of two
or more similarly shaped or similarly sized characters ([comparison:
configurational] or [co-classification]). INTER-EVENT relations account for
how narrative events relate to each other in successive or simultaneous images
as “activity sequences” (Barthes 1977, in Painter et al., p. 73) through
expansion ([unfolding]) and [projection], including options such as
[projection: real/imagined], or [unfolding: succession: +/-cause] and
[+/-fulfilled]. The system also accounts for how the pace of a narrative is
maintained or varied at different points according to differing lengths of
activity sequences. INTER-CIRCUMSTANCE deals with variations in context, by
either maintaining or varying the degree of contextualization in a sequence of
images. The [change context] option, for example, “generally serves to help
stage the story” (p. 82), and Painter et al. note the importance in children’s
picture books of the [change context: home] option as providing a safe and
familiar location ([home: in]) on the one hand, and a space for greater
insecurity and potential excitement ([home: out]) on the other (cf. Nodelman
and Reimer 2003).

The chapter concludes with an analysis of the visual ideational meanings in
Gary Crew and Gregory Rogers’s (1992) ‘Lucy’s Bay.’ As in the previous
chapter, Painter et al. discuss selected instantiations and realizations, and
provide a page-by-page, system-by-system summary of the visual ideational
choices made in this picture book. Several images from ‘Lucy’s Bay’ are
included as examples.

Chapter 4, “Composing Visual Space,” deals with visual textual meanings in
children’s picture books, i.e., the ways in which ideational and interpersonal
meanings relate to each other. The chapter examines visual textual meanings
through three systems: INTERMODAL INTEGRATION, FRAMING, and FOCUS. INTERMODAL
INTEGRATION, as the term suggests, deals with the arrangement and location of
both visual and verbal components. The verbiage here, however, is treated
“purely as a visual unit” (p. 92; cf. Chapter 5), and its relation to the
image is categorized as either [integrated] or [complementary]. Suboptions of
[complementary] include issues of axis ([facing] or [descending]), weight (a
continuum between [image privileged] and [verbiage privileged]), and placement
([adjacent] or [interpolating: verbiage medial/image medial]). Similarly, for
[integrated], the main suboptions are [projected] and [expanded], each of
which is an entry condition to further levels of delicacy, such as [projected:
meaning: locution/idea], realized by verbiage appearing in speech or thought
bubbles, respectively. The basic choices for FRAMING are [bound] or [unbound]
images, with further suboptions available for each. For example, [bound:
framed: experiential frame] is realized by a framing device that is also an
experiential part of the image, such as a climbing frame in Anthony Browne’s
(1998) ‘Voices in the Park’ (Painter et al., p. 108); [unbound:
decontextualized: individuated/localized] distinguishes between an image with
participants and no depicted context (i.e. a white-space background;
[individuated]) and one with minimal setting or symbolic attributes
([localized]). For the FOCUS system, Painter et al. introduce the concept of
“focus group” to define visual elements that constitute “a pulse of
information” (p. 109). They further state that “any picture book layout,
framed by the page edges, constitutes a major focus group with a particular
compositional pattern,” which “may itself encompass further focus groups of
varying prominence” (p. 109-110). The FOCUS system attempts to account for
these different levels of compositional patterns by basically treating them as
either [iterating] or [centrifocal], i.e., as repeated [scattered/aligned] or
balanced on or around an occupied or unoccupied center point. Further
suboptions of [centrifocal] are available, i.e., [centered] and [polarized],
both of which are entry conditions to suboptions of increasing delicacy.

Chapter 4 ends with an analysis of the visual textual meanings in ‘Possum
Magic’ by Mem Fox and Julie Vivas (1989/2004 editions). Painter et al. provide
a descriptive analysis of selected instantiations and realizations, noting
shifts in textual meanings at different generic stages of the story. The
section also includes a table that summarizes and compares the main FOCUS
choices in the two editions of the book. Several schematic diagrams
illustrating these choices are provided.

Chapter 5, “Intermodality: Image and Verbiage,” proposes ways in which the
image analyses of the previous chapters might fruitfully be combined with
verbal analyses in order to study the contributions and interplay of the two
semiotics in children’s picture books. Painter et al. compare their
descriptions of visual meaning systems with the verbal meaning systems
provided by Halliday and Matthiessen (2004) and Martin and Rose (2007),
proposing “commitment” (i.e. the degree of meaning contributed by specific
choices) and “coupling” (i.e. the co-patterning of realizations from two or
more systems) as ways of facilitating comparison of the two semiotics.

The penultimate section of the chapter provides an analysis of verbal-visual
instantiation in Libby Hathorn and Gregory Rogers’s (1994) ‘Way Home.’
Examples are given of the ways in which certain visual and verbal
interpersonal, ideational, and textual meanings converge and diverge
throughout the book, and of how these convergences and divergences invite
readers “to empathise with Shane’s [the homeless protagonist’s] situation
while simultaneously [keeping them] at a safe distance from it” (p. 149).

The authors conclude by reiterating that children’s picture books “are not
only enjoyable for young readers, but offer a very important ‘training’ in
becoming sensitised in how to read narrative texts (including monomodal ones)
in ways that are educationally valued” (p. 156).

EVALUATION

Painter et al.’s systemic-functional approach to describing the meaning-making
resources of children’s picture books is interesting and impressive. Much of
their work builds on ‘Reading Images: The Grammar of Visual Design’ (Kress and
van Leeuwen 2006), and the authors carefully construct their arguments around
those of Kress and van Leeuwen (2006), variously adopting, adapting, and
challenging key concepts from that book, while adding new insights relevant to
the study of visual-verbal narratives.

Painter et al.’s analyses are so rich that a book review of this kind may not
do justice to the concepts and examples they discuss. The visual detail and
descriptive power of system networks, for example, cannot be easily captured
in such a “verbal” review, and their carefully constructed systems, along with
example instantiations and realizations, are undoubtedly one of the book’s
major strengths. In each chapter, the reader is introduced to a series of
meaning systems and then guided through the various levels of delicacy within
each system, with the description of each new feature or option accompanied by
varied examples of its realization. Indeed, these system networks reveal some
potentially interesting theoretical insights, one of which is support for the
general observation that non-verbal semiotics may have more fluid categories
than language (p. 10). Although this may be a matter of representation (cf.
discussion of MODALITY: VALUE as scaled or categorical in Martin & White 2005:
15-16), visual interpersonal meaning systems in particular, e.g., AMBIENCE and
PATHOS, are frequently scaled rather than categorical, and quantification
features and certain CHARACTER APPEARANCE features are presented, by double
brackets, as both alternative and simultaneous options.

With regard to system networks, there seem to be some minor discrepancies in
notation. In the CHARACTER APPEARANCE system (p. 64), for example, the
potentially simultaneous options of [descriptive detail], [symbolic
attribute], and [garment/accessory] have been marked as categorical
alternatives, where only one of the three can be selected. Similarly, some
instantiations have been marked with slashes rather than colons, and vice
versa, where I suspect it was not intended, e.g., in Table 3.7 (pp. 88-89),
where [reappear/immediate: …] suggests that [reappear] and [immediate] are two
simultaneous options at the same level of delicacy (I assume a colon should
have been used). However, these are minor points. Of greater concern, perhaps,
is the fact that all the figures in Painter et al. are in black and white (or
graytone), at least in the edition I have reviewed. This is a drawback in
general, since the use of colors in picture books is referred to at various
points throughout the book, but is most unfortunate in the section on
AMBIENCE. I hope this can be rectified in future editions, or in an e-book
edition, if one is planned.

The back cover notes that this book should be of interest to researchers in
MDA, systemic-functional theory, and children’s literature and literacy. I
agree, while also adding that, despite the book’s focus on children’s picture
books, many of the insights provided in ‘Reading Visual Narratives’ could be
applied to similar visual-verbal narrative forms, such as comics and graphic
novels, magazines, and webpages, to name a few. Researchers and educators
interested in these and similar visual-verbal narratives would be well advised
to read Painter et al.’s book. The integrated approach of Chapter 5 is
particularly useful in this regard, with its discussion of commitment and
coupling, and its identification of key narrative domains. Indeed, the latter
may also be relevant for examining intersemiosis with other semiotic modes,
e.g., sound (see van Leeuwen 1999).

Overall, ‘Reading Visual Narratives’ is an important contribution to the field
of social semiotics, in general, and to the study of visual-verbal narratives
in children’s picture books, in particular. Painter et al. have produced a
work that is (almost) as fascinating as the material upon which it is based.

REFERENCES

Halliday, M. A. K. 1978. Language as social semiotic: the social
interpretation of language and meaning. London: Arnold.

Halliday, M. A. K. and C. M. I. M. Matthiessen. 2004. Introduction to
functional grammar. 3rd edition. London: Arnold.

Kress, Gunther and Theo van Leeuwen. 1996/2006. Reading images: the grammar of
visual design. 1st/2nd edition. Abingdon: Routledge.

Martin, J. R. & David Rose. 2007. Working with discourse: meaning beyond the
clause. 2nd edition. London: Continuum.

Martin, J. R. & P. R. R. White. 2005. The language of evaluation: appraisal in
English. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.

Nodelman, Perry and Mavis Reimer. 2003. The pleasures of children’s
literature. 3rd edition. Boston, Massachusetts: Allyn and Bacon.

Leeuwen, Theo van. 1999. Speech, music, sound. London: Macmillan.

ABOUT THE REVIEWER

Daniel Lees Fryer is a PhD researcher at the University of Gothenburg, Sweden,
and an assistant professor at Oslo and Akershus University College of Applied
Sciences (HiOA), Norway. His research interests include systemic-functional
grammar and social semiotics, academic literacies, and scientific discourse.
He holds courses and workshops in academic writing for staff and students at
HiOA and at the University of Oslo, Norway, and occasional courses in
systemic-functional theory.

Viewpoint in Language: A Multimodal Perspective

No Comments

Editor: Barbara Dancygier
Editor: Eve Sweetser
Title: Viewpoint in Language
Subtitle: A Multimodal Perspective
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Year: 2012

Reviewer: Simone C. Bacchini, The British Library, UK

SUMMARY

The theme explored in this volume is “viewpoint” or “perspective”. The two
terms are very common: the former is often associated with literary and
linguistic studies whilst the latter is familiar in the figurative arts. Both
imply that any act of representation, linguistic or otherwise, necessitates a
sentient entity doing the “viewing” as well as a “viewed”. These observations
are not new but the somewhat novel idea that lies behind this new volume is
that viewpoint is much more pervasive in human cognition and language than has
been hitherto acknowledged. “Subjectivity”, meaning the particular position
and embodied perspective from which a cognitive and encoding (e.g. through
language) act is performed, is crucial and shapes forms of communication.
Furthermore, it can be argued that communication itself (in its broadest
sense) exists to encode and allow the expression of viewpoint. As mentioned,
literary (or narrative) viewpoint has been explored extensively. However, the
volume’s editors argue that more work is needed to explore the relations
between simple and complex viewpoints. In addition, there is also a need to
further explore and question the relationship between physical viewpoint and
more abstract ones, such as the one we find in narrative. “Viewpoint in
Language” takes a multidisciplinary, multimodal approach by bringing together
researchers from different scholarly communities and working within different
fields to explore and highlight the centrality and pervasiveness of viewpoint.

The book is divided into four parts: Part One deals with “intersubjectivity
and subjectification”; Part Two addresses “gesture and processing of visual
information”; Part Three contains contributions on “multiple viewpoints in
American Sign Language; and Part Four deals with “constructions and
discourse”. One of the editors, Eve Sweetser, provides an introduction, whilst
the conclusion, “multiple viewpoints, multiple spaces”, is by the other
editor, Barbara Dancygier. Part one contains three contributions each, whilst
parts two, three, and four contain two each.

The chapters in Part One, “Intersubjectivity and Subjectification”, explore
the ways in which speakers weave complex viewpoints by simultaneously evoking
and appealing to contrasting, even conflicting, spaces.

Chapter 1, by Vera Tobin and Michael Israel, offers a novel analysis of irony,
both situational and literary. They argue that irony naturally follows from
the narrative mind, from the possibility of encoding anything that we
encounter, both as something which simply occurs and something that is
represented, thus relying on viewpoint. The authors argue that irony is
pervasive but that its functioning and interpretation are cognitively
demanding processes. They rely on the ability to connect with a single
all-viewing mental space, i.e., an “all-knowing viewer”. The authors argue for
the existence of close and natural relations between different types of
“verbal, situational, and structural ironies” (p. 44), which make it difficult
to explain why irony is often unsettling.

Chapter 2 is by Lilian Ferrari and Eve Sweetser. It offers an analysis of
historical processes of semantic subjectification by resorting to the notion
of viewpoint relations within a complex and dynamic network of mental spaces.
Among the examples they deal with are the cases of deictic markers morphing
into articles and the emergence of epistemic meanings from deontic ones
encoded by modals. They argue that the result of this inclusion reveals higher
subjectivity, since the incorporated meanings are located in higher mental
spaces, further apart from the real-world content being described.

Chapter 3, by Barbara Dancygier, deals with concepts of negation in the
context of the mental space framework (MSF). By developing the concept of an
“alternativity of negation”, the author argues that negation is a device for
marking viewpoint and signalling stance. The author considers some
constructions that involve negation in order to explain its intersubjective
role and shows how this is used to negotiate multiple viewpoints which are
made available by a specific context. This approach, the author argues, can
help clarify problematic areas such as the interpretation of Neg-Raising and
metalinguistic negation.

Chapter 4, by Fey Parrill, is concerned with viewpoint in multimodal language,
i.e., “speech and speech accompanying gestures” (p. 97) (if the version in the
text contains single quotes, please put double quotes around the single
quotes). The author separates viewpoint into three distinct — although
interconnected — phenomena: conceptual, linguistic, and gestural. She argues
that considering viewpoint as seen in co-gesture can help bring together and
harmonise general notions of viewpoint and views of it as understood by the
‘blending and conceptual integration framework’, a theory of cognition
according to which elements from various scenarios are subconsciously
‘blended’. According to the theory, this process underlies thinking and speech
processes. The author describes an experimental study involving twenty-four
university students. They were each accompanied by a friend and, after
watching three cartoon stimuli, had to describe them to their friend. Each of
the participants watched the cartoon in one of two conditions: the ‘shared
knowledge condition’ and the ‘control condition’. In the former, the
participant watched the stimuli with his/her friend, while in the latter
he/she watched alone. The study helps to shed light on the ways in which the
two modalities of speech and gestures are connected.

Chapter 5, by Shweta Narayan, continues the exploration of gesture in
conversation, although this is done within the context of spoken language
rather than signed language, as in the previous chapter. In this chapter, the
author shows how interactants create meaning collaboratively thanks to their
ability to shift viewpoints as evidence of erroneous interpretations of
previous interactions emerge. By looking at how interactants “align” their
gestures, she is able to show that they are able to show viewpoint, thus
cognitively aligning themselves. with their interactants.

Chapter 6, by Barbara Shaffer, is a further contribution to our understanding
of sign language (specifically, American Sign Language (ASL)) from the point
of view of Cognitive Grammar.. The author analyses one of the ways in which
ASL users incorporate viewpoint in their discourse. In particular, she looks
at how reported speech is marked in ASL and concludes that the ways in which
evidentiality is marked and grammaticised in ASL is in many aspects similar to
the ways this is done in spoken language.

Chapter 7, by Terry Janzen, investigates yet another aspect of ASL discourse.
He describes a strategy used by ASL users that depends on imagining a
180-degree rotation of the signer’s body. This allows him or her to alternate
between physically representing one of the participants in a reported
conversation, and then the interlocutor, who is facing him/her. It is this
shift in imagined viewpoint, the author argues, which is based on the
interactants’ cooperation, that enables the correct interpretation of messages
and thus allows effective communication.

Chapter 8 is by Niki Nikiforidou. Leaving signed language behind, the opening
contribution to the last section of the volume brings the discussion back to
more familiar grounds, namely, narrative viewpoint in literary texts.
Nikiforidou explores the use, in English, of the past tense with a proximal
deictic ‘now’ (e.g. ‘they were NOW listening to him attentively’). She argues
that its implications are twofold. First, it signals a change of perspective
from ‘outside’ to ‘within’ the narrated event. Secondly, this narrative
strategy should be viewed on par with other discourse-grammatical
constructions of a high-level type.

Chapter 9 is by Lieven Vandelanotte. In it, the author argues that a more
nuanced account of reported speech and thought phenomena is possible — and,
in fact, desirable — if the area between direct and indirect speech (or
thought) is not considered as belonging to a single area of free indirect
forms. The author identifies a separate type of indirect speech: “distancing
direct speech and thought” (p. 198). This maintains the deictic centre and
particular viewpoint aligned with the quoting speaker, whilst incorporating
the quoted speaker’s speech.

The volume concludes with a chapter by Barbara Dancygier called ‘Multiple
viewpoints, multiple spaces’. In it, the author draws together the various
strands explored in the volume and one again highlights how, contrary to what
some might think, the linguistic and gestural structures examined by the
various contributors are far from simple. She also suggests further avenues of
exploration, such as more detailed explorations of the “mechanisms yielding
the configurations of viewpoint” (p. 228) addressed by the contributors to the
volume.

EVALUATION

Due to its multidisciplinary approach and multimodal orientation, ‘Viewpoint
in Language’ is likely to appeal to diverse audiences, most likely at the
post-graduate level. Indeed, a fundamental strength of the volume is that it
coherently brings together strands of research that are frequently pursued
separately.

The various contributors to this volume convincingly show the centrality of
viewpoint in human cognition and its ubiquity across a range of communicative
modes. Crucially, they also show to what extent human cognition and
communication are profoundly embodied phenomena.

The section on signed communication is — in this reviewer’s opinion —
important and effective in this respect. Perhaps because of their
‘physicality’, or their more obviously embodied nature, signed languages can
be especially useful in highlighting interlocutors’ need (and ability) to take
into account other people’s — as well as their own — unique viewpoint in the
encoding, decoding, and transmitting of a linguistic message. Although studies
of signed languages are by no means rare, the existing ones still do no
justice to the importance of close analysis of signed languages, both for its
numerical relevance in terms of users, and for its relevance for a deeper
understanding of both cognitive and communicative processes.

The editors of this volume acknowledge that the study of viewpoint, and an
appreciation of its pervasiveness in human communication, are not new. What
this collection of essays adds is a clear example of the ways in which a
multimodal approach enhances our understanding of the cognitive processes
involved in communication. This is because such an approach is better at
picking up clues that would not be entirely accessible if only a single mode
— such as the study of written literary fiction, for example — is
considered. One of the major strengths of this volume, therefore, is that it
deals with the implications and manifestation of subjective viewpoint
holistically, as shown by the aforementioned sections on American Sign
Language.

The papers published in this volume are not informed by new theories. As the
editors say it is “a contribution to the study of language in the context of
embodied (or grounded) cognition” (p. 3). It makes use of the MSF and, more
generally, insights gained from Cognitive Grammar. However, the papers
presented in the volume present new case-studies or revisit topics that have
been the object of considerable attention in a new light. Such is the case
with irony (Tobin and Israel, pp. 25-46), viewed as a viewpoint phenomenon and
as a “figure of subjectivity” (p. 44).

The final section of the volume (Constructions and Discourse, pp. 177-218)
will be of particular interest to, and — in this reviewer’s opinion – greatly
appreciated by, literary scholars. As stated, literary narrative is perhaps
the field that has had the longest familiarity with viewpoint and
subjectivity. However, Nikiforidou’s and Vandelanotte’s essays bring a more
nuanced examination of the linguistic machinery that underpins the encoding of
viewpoint in narrative and literary texts. Like the other contributions, but
perhaps even more so — given the aforementioned familiarity of literary and
narrative studies with viewpoint — these two essays exemplify how the
interdisciplinary and multimodal approach exemplified in, and exemplified by,
this volume truly represents a step forward in our understanding of
subjectivity.

The ability with which the editors have been able to give shape to a coherent
approach is to be applauded. It is likely that a number of researchers now
working separately in different areas on viewpoint will be encouraged to carry
on with their work enlightened and inspired by this volume. Indeed, the
bringing together of linguistics, cognitive science, and literary studies by a
unifying concept of subjectivity as an embodied phenomenon is a major and
needed achievement.

About the Reviewer:
Simone Bacchini has recently been awarded a PhD in linguistics, having
defended a thesis on the linguistic encoding of the experiences of physical
pain and chronic illness through the lexicogrammar of Italian. His research
interests include sociolinguistic, Systemic Functional Grammar and discourse
analysis. As a result of his doctoral research, he has developed and interest
in health communication and the use of language in medicine and medical
settings. He is currently researching the encoding of ‘affect’ in
doctor-patient communication, with particular attention to the role of the
interpreter in situations when medical professionals and patients do not speak
the same language.

Newer Entries

Get Adobe Flash player